Interactive Narrative Design Beyond the Secret Art Status: A Method to Verify Design Conventions for Interactive Narrative
Hartmut Koenitz
Christian Roth
Teun Dubbelman
Noam Knoller
HKU UNIVERSITY OF THE ARTS, UTRECHT
I. Introduction
A decade after the narratology vs. ludology debate, game narrative has emerged as an area for novel game concepts and as a strategy to distinguish a particular title. The critical and economic success of Telltale, with titles such as The Walking Dead (Telltale Games, 2012) , The Wolf Amongst us (Telltale Games, 2013) or Minecraft: Story Mode (Telltale Games, 2015), but also of independent productions from Save the Date (Cornell, 2013) to The Stanley Parable (Wreden, 2011) and Firewatch (Campo Santo, 2016) is testimony to this development. At the same time, this demonstrable interest by audiences is not met by a wider body of specialized knowledge and training in narrative-focused game design.
Game design programs might include a course in game narrative, but such an effort barely scratches the surface of a complex topic. At the time of this writing, no degree program in interactive narrative design exists. Many existing practitioners are self-trained and often rely on intuition and experimental approaches in their design practice. We find innovation to be coming from noted designers like David Cage (Fahrenheit/Indigo Prophecy, Heavy Rain (Quantic Dream, 2010) ) and midsize game companies like Telltale (which was much smaller when it invented the now famous “telltale formula”). The latter’scase is instructive, as the company slowly built up, in an iterative process of continuous refinement, to the narrative design formula that had made The Walking Dead such an outstanding success. However, there are already signs that the ‘Telltale formula’ has become stale, since — in the eyes of some critics and players — later titles provide new content, but only minor additions to the overall design approach (Sinha, 2016; S. Wright, 2015) . This lack of development comes as no surprise, since narrative-focused game design is a double secret art. First, it is practiced only by relatively few ‘initiated’ self-trained practitioners. Second, where more knowledge has been accumulated in the commercial space, it is treated as a closely-guarded company secret. Clearly, knowledge about narrative game design exists (as Dubbelman (2016) reminds us), and it is disseminated at places like the GDC (Game Developer Conference) narrative summit. However, as we have identified earlier, there is a lack of professional training (Koenitz 2014) further compounded by the fact that much of the available knowledge is either too abstract (e.g. high-level concepts like Murray’s “Scripting the interactor” (Murray, 1997) ) or too specific (valid only for particular works as discussed in post mortems at the GDC narrative summit) (Koenitz, 2015). This state of affairs is an impediment to overall development in this area of game design and related fields such as interactive documentaries, journalistic ‘interactives’and installation pieces. What is missing is an established body of design conventions for game narrative that fulfill a similar role to our more developed understanding of theatrical or cinematic conventions.
Interestingly, a look back at the early years of video games shows that narrative game design is not exactly a new idea. Pioneering text-based games like Adventure (Crowther, 1976) lead to the creation of the IF genre including successful narrative-focused commercial games like Zork (Lebling, 1980; Lebling, Blank, & Anderson, 1979) and Planetfall (Infocom, 1983). However, the rejection of narrative as an avenue for video games in the narratology vs. ludology debate (Aarseth, 2001; 2004; Eskelinen, 2001; Frasca, 1999; Juul, 1999; 2001) , in concert with a disregard for the specific needs of interactive narrative design (e.g. Jesse Schell’s rant against specific approaches in Fullerton’s Game Design Workshop (Fullerton, Swain, & Hoffman, 2008) ) has resulted in a scarcity of efforts to analyze and develop narrative game design.
There is also a connection between the minor status of educational efforts in game narrative and the lack of established interactive narrative design conventions. Education depends on the availability of teaching materials. Our work is therefore intended to have impact in both education and practical application by existing practitioners. We describe a method to verify narrative design conventions using empirical methods. Instead of the trial-and-error process of old, our method evaluates user reaction to A/B prototypes using an established toolkit measuring immersion, agency and transformation through a granular set of 13 psychological dimensions (Roth and Koenitz, 2016; Roth and Vermeulen, 2013; Roth, Klimmt, Vermeulen, and Vorderer, 2011; Roth, Vorderer, Klimmt, and Vermeulen, 2010) .
In the remainder of the paper, we discuss the notion of design conventions and related work before describing the measurement toolbox we use and the process for verification. Finally, we explain the concrete setup and draw the connection to education.
II. Design conventions
The concept of guidelines for the creation of narrative artifacts might be traced back to early guides on literary composition. The introduction of conventions is a process whereby authors or designers experiment with formal patterns that can carry cultural meaning, audiences indicate whether these patterns are indeed perceived as intended, and through iteration (Derrida, 1988) they become conventional. In cinema, for example, many conventions now taught in film schools — for example continuity editing in Hollywood cinema (Thompson and Bordwell, 2012) — have historically evolved over a period of several decades of trial and error. Therefore, it might seem that conventions cannot be imposed on the field. They become accepted design conventions once both authors and audiences have appropriated them, and they become implicit cultural knowledge. However, some conventions have been the result of experimental setups and quasi-academic investigations, for example by pioneering Russian filmmaker Lev Kuleshov, who devised a method to establish the effects of montage on viewers (Kuleshov, 1974) . Kuleshov interjected the same shot of an expressionless actor between different images (e.g. of food or a funeral) and was able to demonstrate that audiences perceived the actor's expression differently depending on associations originating from the context provided by the shots that surrounded them. These experiments provided the basis of the Soviet montage technique of film editing, as theorized by Sergey Eisenstein (first in lectures, then from 1937 in written publications (Eisenstein, 2010) ).
Scholarly methods in user experience research (Calvillo Gámez, Cairns, and Cox, 2009; Dow, 2007; Hassenzahl, Diefenbach, and Göritz, 2010; Roth, Vermeulen, Vorderer, and Klimmt, 2012a; P. Wright and McCarthy, 2008; Zammitto, Mirza-Babaei, Livingston, Kobayashi, and Nacke, 2014) now provide the means to analyze the effects of particular design approaches, and thus to identify and evaluate candidate conventions empirically, without the need for extensive trial and error. In other words, design conventions can be evaluated through their impact on user experiences and this is the area we focus on.
III. Related work
Comparative methods for the investigation of user experiences in interactive narrative are well established. Dow et al. (Dow, Mehta, Harmon, MacIntyre, and Mateas, 2007) examined user experiences across different versions of the interactive drama Façade. Participants used an augmented reality version (in which a see-through display projects the characters into the physical recreation of the apartment), a desktop version with speech communication, and a desktop version using a keyboard input. Through interviews and observations, the authors found that immersive augmented reality can increase perceived presence.
Aylett et al. (Aylett, Louchart, Dias, Paiva, and Vala, 2005) conducted a small-scale (N = 11) user test with the emergent Interactive Storytelling system FearNot!, and collected children’s responses on a short set of evaluation items. They investigated whether conversations were perceived as interesting and ‘felt real,’ as well as how autonomous characters seemed to respond to user input (e.g., whether they seemed to be listening to user advice). In comparison to a scripted version of the same setting — featuring non-autonomous characters — a larger-scaled study (Hall et al., 2004) showed that character responsiveness in the emergent software was experienced as less real, less interesting, and less responsive to user input. The authors concluded that the conversations in the scripted version were perceived as more coherent than the conversations with autonomous characters, which sometimes seemed unresponsive to user advice.
These earlier studies have been useful in optimizing system parameters and creating more effective links between the goals of a given interactive narrative and user requirements. However, the measurements applied in these studies do not enable the systematic testing and comparison of design conventions. Roth’s approach (Roth et al., 2010) overcomes this limitation and enables us to empirically compare candidates for design conventions. Based on Entertainment Theory (Bryant and Vorderer, 2013) , Roth’s measurement toolbox covers a wide range of user experience dimensions (effectance, autonomy, usability, suspense, curiosity, presence, identification, character believability, flow, eudaimonic appreciation, enjoyment, positive and negative affect). We have recently mapped these dimensions (Roth and Koenitz, 2016) to Murray’s more broadly understood concepts (Murray, 1997): agency, immersion, and transformation.
This measurement toolbox has been applied within a range of different studies (Roth et al., 2010; Roth and Vermeulen, 2013; Roth et al., 2012b) and proved to be useful for the evaluation of interactive digital narrative (IDN) user experiences. For example, a prototype-based user study compared a turn-based dialogue design to a real-time approach (Endrass, Klimmt, Mehlmann, André, and Roth, 2011) . Findings suggest that participants value real-time dialog more highly in terms of perceived autonomy and elicited curiosity. Another study (Roth et al., 2012b) evaluated the effects of different player modes (embodied actor mode versus disembodied ghost mode) on the user experience (sense of immersion vs. control). The results confirmed the assumption that control-related experiences such as autonomy, effectance, and flow, ranked higher in disembodied ghost mode, resulting in a higher sense of accomplishment and user satisfaction. Another study investigated the effect of auditory feedback on user enjoyment of an interactive surround video. In this A/B comparison, participants watched two variants of an interactive movie on the virtual reality hardware Oculus Rift, either with or without sound feedback as rewarding indicator for successful interactions. The authors had hypothesized that auditory feedback would increase the perception of effectance and had to reject this assumption (Vosmeer, Roth, and Schouten, 2015) . Instead, interactors rated presence significantly lower as the sounds hampered their feeling of immersion. These studies show the validity of user experience testing when considering design decisions.
IV. Verifying design conventions
In our approach towards the verification of design conventions, we apply experimental designs and methods from psychological research. The design of our experimental setup is based on earlier experience with IDN user experience evaluation (Roth and Vermeulen, 2013) , with prototype-based user studies (Endrass et al., 2011) and with the A/B comparison approach (Roth et al., 2012b) .
Design conventions can be verified through their impact on the user experience. There are two broad approaches (Bernhaupt, 2010) . First, there is the evaluation of hardware technology and the software itself, in terms of user interface usability, which investigates general system responsiveness and usability. Second, there is an approach which focuses on the user experience as the overall appreciation and enjoyment of the experience delivered by the system, e.g. regarding the presentation of the story world, the characters, and the interactions. In respect to the latter, our specific approach is grounded in Entertainment Theory (Bryant and Vorderer, 2013; Vorderer and Reinecke, 2015; Vorderer, Klimmt, and Ritterfeld, 2004) . In this context, ‘entertainment’ — in line with psychological research and the communication sciences — is used to mean the evaluation and experience from the audience’s perspective. ‘Entertainment’ as we use it, is thus not the opposite of ‘artistic value,’ but inclusive of it.
In the current research, we investigate both dimensions — the impact of design decisions in terms of both usability and overall appreciation. This kind of hybrid enjoyment is a highly complex experiential state with a variety of manifestations (e.g. exhilaration, suspense, and identification) and numerous determinants attached to both the system delivering the experience and the person perceiving the system. To evaluate whether and how a given IDN system elicits enjoyment and meaning in users, it is necessary to conceptualize in advance the kind of experiential qualities a system might deliver. Subsequently, experimental exposure studies with control groups aim to measure these experiences and thus help identify what elements determine the appreciation of interactive narrative systems. In this context, the aforementioned toolbox (Roth, 2016) provides the necessary conceptualization and granular measure-ments to empirically evaluate the effectiveness of design strategies.
V. Identifying candidate conventions
We start the process by identifying candidates for verification as design conventions. We use existing scholarly publications as sources for conventions, but also structured interviews with practitioners, expert statements and analysis of artifacts. For example, Janet Murray, in her investigation of Weizenbaum’s early AI therapist simulation Eliza (Weizenbaum, 1966) , identifies the — possibly unconscious — design approach of “scripting the interactor” (Murray, 1997) as a major factor why this early experiment was compelling. Another candidate design convention was identified by Kevin Bruner, CEO of Telltale Games, who stated that an important aspect of his company’s success with narrative-focused games is in using “choice notifications” that alert players of potential later consequences. A further possible convention is “cross-session memory.” This candidate was gained through analysis of the game Save The Date (Cornell, 2013), in which the player experiences the consequences of decisions made in play sessions completed earlier.
VI. User study design
The design of our experimental setup is based on earlier experience with IDN user experience evaluation (Roth and Vermeulen, 2013) , for example, with prototype-based user studies comparing two design alternatives (Endrass et al., 2011; Roth et al., 2012b) . Therefore, we create prototype pairs — one applying a specific design strategy and another omitting it.
In this way, we enable the double-blind two-group post-test-only randomized experimental design, which is an established quantitative research approach for assessing cause-effect relationships (Campbell and Stanley, 1963; Shadish, Cook, and Campbell, 2002). It is one of the best research methods to evaluate the comparative effects of two treatments, in our case the experience of an interactive digital narrative in two versions, differing only in regards to one specific design convention candidate (experimental group featuring the convention candidate and control group exposed to a version without it). In the field of online user experience research, this experimental two-sample hypothesis testing approach to measuring the effects of design alternatives is called “split testing” or “A/B testing.”
Following best practices for these kinds of user studies, we assign participants randomly to the two groups. This procedure leads to a probabilistically equivalent distribution of participants to the two experimental conditions. In every evaluation, we take participants’ demographics, game preferences, previous experience with IND, and other possible mediators into account. In this way, we are able to control the equivalence of the groups and can be used to counterbalance a possible distribution bias when measured prior to the distribution.
An extended version of this user study design, the Randomized-Multigroup Design (Bordens and Abbott, 2002) , introduces one or more additional levels of the independent variable. In our case these would be variants of a design convention candidate. This extended setup would allow for the differentiation of different measurement ‘levels,’ e.g. of agency (a lot influence on a narrative level vs. a bit influence vs. no influence). This would result in a A/B1/…/Bn testing setup. A downside of this setup is the requirement of larger samples, as each added condition multiplies the necessary amount of participants.
VII. Practical setup
Our practical setup involves student projects with a duration of two to three months, with weekly supervision meetings and extensive project documentation. We use regular meetings to discuss suitable narrative design approaches, to play-test, and to check on progress and potential issues. In-between these meetings, students upload their prototypes, work plan (containing milestones) as well as their project diary so they can be evaluated online at any time (e.g. using Google Drive).
Every project starts with a brainstorming phase, resulting in a concept draft. Before using any digital software, a paper prototype of the interactive digital narrative is built and play-tested together with the student. By doing so, potential issues with interaction design, pacing, narrative structure and the use of variables can be identified and solved without spending time on rewriting an application. Once the paper prototype is deemed satisfactory, students recreate it using the ASAPS authoring tool (Koenitz and Chen, 2012) , a software suite for the creation of 2D multiplatform interactive narratives.
VIII. ASAPS software
We use the ASAPS authoring tool (Figure 1) for three reasons: a) the focus on 2D removes the complexity of 3D production and model making and thus enables rapid production of prototypes; b) all elements of the artifact are easily accessible for analysis in a non-proprietary format; c) the software allows for optional user tracking, providing an opportunity for additional data collection and subsequent scholarly treatment.
Figure 1. ASAPS Software architecture.
IX. Educational application
One application domain for the results of this kind of work is in education, as an important part in the professional training of interactive narrative designers. However, this still leaves open the question of how to best disseminate this knowledge. How can we educate a wider group of interactive narrative designers — Janet Murray’s “cyberbards”(Murray, 1997) — so that they understand the demands and possibilities at the intersection of interaction and narration (Koenitz and Louchart, 2015; Spierling and Szilas, 2009) ? The challenge lies in the difference (Mateas, 2010) : interactive narrative design is fundamentally different from approaches in well-established narrative practices, like filmmaking or creative writing; it demands practitioners to be able to design and develop interactive systems aimed to create narrative experiences. Therefore, successful IDN design requires a broad understanding of interaction and engaging narration. One promising approach for the education of IDN authors lies in teaching best practices. This, as we mentioned above, is where the lack of a canon of design conventions becomes also a pedagogical challenge. Therefore, the identification of generalizable IDN design conventions >(Koenitz, 2015; Murray, 2012) becomes a necessary precondition for a more formal educational approach to interactive narrative design.
X. Pedagogical implementation
Once the material for teaching is available, the next question would be: how to embed the knowledge into a concrete pedagogical effort and effectively teach IDN design conventions?
The practice of IDN can be situated within the field of creative technologies; this interdisciplinary and transdisciplinary field within the creative industries is technology-driven, user-oriented, and includes creative practices like interactive documentaries, game development and interaction design. Pedagogical practices in the domain of creative technologies (CT) generally favor practice-based education, contextual authenticity and playful learning (Connor, Marks, and Walker, 2015) . In short, CT pedagogy favors learning by doing in authentic scenarios. Essential skills, attitudes and knowledge are appropriated by learners in situations that recreate the conditions where specific skills, attitudes and knowledge become useful, meaningful and applicable (Maddrell, 2015) .
Following this insight, IDN design conventions can best be taught when a learning situation does not simply offer the conventions to the learner, but incorporates them in the actual process of designing and developing IDN artifacts. In most disciplines in the domain of creative technologies, this process is iterative, and relies on cycles of ideation, prototyping and testing (Meinel, Leifer, and Plattner, 2011) . Learners appropriate the conventions most effectively when they are engaged in the activity of creating new ideas, building prototypes and performing user tests.
XI. Future work
For now, our investigation focuses on universal IDN design conventions. However, design conventions that can be identified in one IDN artifact might not necessarily be applicable to others. We should therefore be careful not to approach IDN design conventions as essentially universal and at least allow that they might be context sensitive and thus work best within the contextual constraints of particular formats, from interactive video games to interactive documentaries. This hypothesis is supported by a comparison with non-interactive media: some narrative conventions in a particular cinematic genre (e.g. horror movies) are applicable to other genres (e.g. romantic comedies) or even other narrative media, while others are not. Likewise, only some design conventions for interactive video may be applicable to video games and vice versa. A next step in our investigation will be an attempt to identify conventions in specific domains and thus enable a distinction between general conventions and ones that are more specific.
Furthermore, we plan to extend the scope of our evaluation methods to larger field tests. By offering some of the prototypes online, we can reach a wider audience and thus further improve the quality of our results in evaluating IDN design convention candidates. Future experimental designs could also include multiple variations, and thus allow for more complex experimental designs.
XII. Conclusion
In this paper, we described an approach for identifying and verifying design conventions for video games narrative. We discussed the nature and emergence of conventions in traditional media, before presenting our methodology to verify conventions by applying the measurements of user experience dimensions to A/B prototypes. Finally, we considered educational applications of the results of our process and future work.
The identification and verification of potential design conventions is an important step towards a specific pedagogy and a more accessible professional and artistic practice — the end of the secret art and the beginning of a professional discipline of interactive narrative designers.
REFERENCES
AARSETH, Espen J. (2001). “Computer Game Studies, Year One.” Game Studies, 1.1. http://gamestudies.org/0101/editorial.html
–––––––––– (2004). “Genre Trouble.” First Person: New Media as Story, Performance, and Game. Ed. Noah Wardrip-Fruin and Pat Harrigan. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. www.electronicbookreview.com/thread/firstperson/vigilant
AYLETT, Ruth S., Sandy Louchart, João Dias, Ana Paiva, and Marco Vala (2005). “FearNot! – An Experiment in Emergent Narrative.” Intelligent Virtual Agents. Ed. Themis Panayiotopoulos, Jonathan Gratch, Ruth Aylett, Daniel Ballin, Patrick Olivier, and Thomas Rist. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 3661: 305–316. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. http://doi.org/10.1007/11550617_26
BERNHAUPT, Regina (2010). “User Experience Evaluation in Entertainment.” Evaluating User Experience in Games: Concepts and Methods. Ed. Regina Bernhaupt. London: Springer London. 3-7. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84882-963-3_1
BORDENS, Kenneth S., and Bruce Barrington Abbott. (2002). Research Design and Methods: A Process Approach (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.
BRYANT, Jennings, and Peter Vorderer, eds. (2013). Psychology of Entertainment. London: Routledge.
CALVILLO GÁMEZ, Eduardo H., Paul Cairns, and Anna L. Cox. (2009). “From the Gaming Experience to the Wider User Experience.” Proceedings of the 23rd British HCI Group Annual Conference on People and Computers: Celebrating People and Technology, British Computer Society. 520–523.
CAMPO SANTO. (2016). Firewatch [Video Game]. Portland, OR: Panic.
CONNOR, Andy M., Stefan Marks, and Charles Walker. (2015). “Creating Creative Technologists: Playing With(in) Education.” Creativity in the Digital Age. Ed. Nelson Zagalo, and Pedro Branco. London: Springer London. 35–56. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-6681-8_3
CORNELL, Chris (2013). Save the Date. Paper Dino Software.
CROWTHER, William, and Don Woods (1976). Adventure [Video Game].
DERRIDA, Jacques (1988). “Signature Event Context.” Limited Inc. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. 1-23.
DOW, Steven (2007). “User Engagement in Physically Embodied Narrative Experiences.” Proceedings of the 6th ACM SIGCHI Conference on Creativity and Cognition. New York: ACM. 280-280. 10.1145/1254960.1255016
DOW, Steven, Manish Mehta, Ellie Harmon, Blair MacIntyre, and Michael Mateas (2007). “Presence and Engagement in an Interactive Drama.” CHI 07: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, New York: ACM. 1475-1484. http://doi.org/10.1145/1240624.1240847
DUBBELMAN, Teun (2016). “Narrative Game Mechanics.” International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling – ICIDS 2016. Ed. Frank Nack and Andrew S. Gordon. Springer International Publishing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 10045: 39-50. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-48279-8_4
EISENSTEIN, Sergei (2010). Towards a Theory of Montage. London: I.B. Tauris.
ENDRASS, Birgit, Christoph Klimmt, Gregor Mehlmann, Elisabeth André, and Christian Roth (2011). “Exploration of User Reactions to Different Dialog-Based Interaction Styles.” International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling – ICIDS 2011 (Vol. 7069, pp. 243–248). Ed. Mei Si, David Thue, Elisabeth André, James C. Lester, Joshua Tanenbaum, and Veronica Zammitto. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 7069: 243–248. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-25289-1_26
ESKELINEN, Markku (2001). “The gaming situation.” Game Studies, 1.1. http://www.gamestudies.org/0101/eskelinen/
FRASCA, Gonzalo (1999). “Ludology meets narratology: similitude and differences between (video) games and narrative.” 3 Dec 2017. http://www.ludology.org/articles/ludology.htm
FULLERTON, Tracy, Christopher Swain, and Steven S. Hoffman. (2008). Game Design Workshop: A Playcentric Approach to Creating Innovative Games. Burlington, MA: Morgan Kaufmann (2nd ed.).
HALL, Lynne, Sarah Woods, Kerstin Dautenhahn, Daniel Sobral, Ana Paiva, Dieter Wolke, and Lynne Newall (2004). “Designing Empathic Agents: Adults versus Kids.” Intelligent Tutoring Systems. Ed. James C. Lester, Rosa Maria Vicari, and Fábio Paraguaçu. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 3220: 604–613. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-30139-4_57
HASSENZAHL, Marc, Sarah Diefenbach, and Anja Göritz (2010). “Needs, affect, and interactive products: Facets of user experience.” Interacting with Computers 22.5: 353–362. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intcom.2010.04.002
INFOCOM (1983). Planetfall [Video Game]. Cambridge, MA: Infocom.
JUUL, Jesper (1999). “A clash between game and narrative.” Danish Literature.
–––––––––– (2001). “Games telling stories? A brief note on games and narratives,” Game Studies 1.1 http://www.gamestudies.org/0101/juul-gts/
KOENITZ, Hartmut (2014). “Five Theses for Interactive Digital Narrative.” Interactive Storytelling: 7th International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS 2014, Singapore, Singapore, November 3-6, 2014, Proceedings. Ed. Clara Fernández-Vara, Alex Mitchell, and D. Thue. Cham: Springer International Publishing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science Vol. 8832; 134–139: http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12337-0_13
–––––––––– (2015). “Design Approaches for Interactive Digital Narrative.” Interactive Storytelling: 8th International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS 2015, Copenhagen, Denmark, November 30 - December 4, 2015, Proceedings. Ed. Henrik Schoenau-Fog, Luis Emilio Bruni, Sandy Louchart, and Sarune Baceviciute. Cham: Springer International Publishing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science Vol. 9445: 50–57. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-27036-4_5
KOENITZ, Hartmut, and Kun-Ju Chen (2012). “Genres, Structures and Strategies in Interactive Digital Narratives – Analyzing a Body of Works Created in ASAPS.” Interactive Storytelling: 5th International Conference, ICIDS 2012, San Sebastián, Spain, November 12-15, 2012. Proceedings. Ed. David Oyarzun, Federico Peinado, R. Michael Young, Ane Elizalde, and Gonzalo Méndez. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 7648: 84–95. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34851-8_8
KOENITZ, Hartmut, and Sandy Louchart (2015). “Practicalities and Ideologies: (Re)-Considering the Interactive Digital Narrative Authoring Paradigm.” Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on the Foundations of Digital Games (FDG 2015). Eds. Boyang Li, and Mark Nelson. http://www.fdg2015.org/papers/fdg2015_paper_03.pdf
KULESHOV, Lev V. (1974). Kuleshov on Film: Writings. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.
LEBLING, Peter David (1980). Zork [Video Game]. Cambridge, MA: Infocom.
LEBLING, Peter David, Marc Blank, and Tim Anderson. (1979). “Special Feature Zork: A Computerized Fantasy Simulation Game.” Computer 12.4: 51–59. http://doi.org/10.1109/MC.1979.1658697
MADDRELL, Jennifer A. (2015). “Designing Authentic Educational Experiences through Virtual Service Learning.” The Design of Learning Experience. Ed. Brad Hokanson, Gregory Clinton, and Monica W. Tracey. Cham: Springer International Publishing. 215–229. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16504-2_15
MATEAS, Michael (2010). “The Authoring Challenge in Interactive Storytelling.” Interactive Storytelling: Third Joint Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling. Ed. Ruth Aylett, Mei Yii Lim, Sandy Louchart, Paolo Petta, and Mark Riedl. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 6432: 1–1. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-16638-9_1
MEINEL, Christoph, Larry Leifer, and Hasso Plattner, eds. (2011). Design Thinking: Understand, Improve, Apply. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-13757-0
MURRAY, Janet H. (1997). Hamlet on the Holodeck: the Future of Narrative in Cyberspace. New York: Free Press.
–––––––––– (2012). Inventing the Medium: Principles of Interaction Design as a Cultural Practice. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
QUANTIC DREAM. (2010). Heavy Rain [Video game]. Tokyo: Sony Computer Entertainment.
ROTH, Christian (2016). Experiencing Interactive Storytelling. Amsterdam: Vrije Universiteit. PhD Thesis.
ROTH, Christian, and Hartmut Koenitz (2016). “Evaluating the User Experience of Interactive Digital Narrative.” AltMM '16 Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on Multimedia Alternate Realities. New York, New York, USA: ACM Press. 31-36. http://doi.org/10.1145/2983298.2983302
ROTH, Christian, and Ivar Vermeulen (2013). “Breaching Interactive Storytelling’s Implicit Agreement: A Content Analysis of Fac̨ade User Behaviors.” H. Koenitz, G. Ferri, M. Haar, D. Sezen, T. I. Sezen, & G. C̨atak (Eds.),Interactive Storytelling: 6th International Conference, ICIDS 2013, Istanbul, Turkey, November 6-9, 2013, Proceedings. Ed.Hartmut Koenitz, Tonguc Ibrahim Sezen, Gabriele Ferri, Mads Haar, Digdem Sezen, Tonguc Ibrahim Sezen, and Güven C̨atak. Cham: Springer International Publishing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 8230: 168–173. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-02756-2_20
ROTH, Christian, Christoph Klimmt, Ivar Vermeulen, and Peter Vorderer (2011). “The Experience of Interactive Storytelling: Comparing ‘Fahrenheit’ with ‘Façade’.” Entertainment Computing – ICEC 2011. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 6972: 13–21. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-24500-8_2
ROTH, Christian, Ivar Vermeulen, Peter Vorderer, and Christoph Klimmt (2012a). “Exploring Replay Value: Shifts and Continuities in User Experiences Between First and Second Exposure to an Interactive Story.” CyberPsychology, Behavior & Social Networking15.7: 378–381. http://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0437
ROTH, Christian, Ivar Vermeulen, Peter Vorderer, Christoph Klimmt, David Pizzi, Jean-Luc Lugrin, and Marc Cavazza (2012b). “Playing In or Out of Character: User Role Differences in the Experience of Interactive Storytelling.” CyberPsychology, Behavior & Social Networking 15.11: 630–633. http://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0621
ROTH, Christian, Peter Vorderer, Christoph Klimmt, and Ivar Vermeulen (2010). “Measuring the user experience in narrative-rich games: towards a concept-based assessment for interactive stories.” Proceedings of the Workshop on Using Game Design and Affective Evaluation to improve User Experience and User Research at Interaktive Kulturen 2010. Duisburg. https://research.vu.nl/en/publications/measuring-the-user-experience-in-narrative-rich-games-towards-a-c
SINHA, Ravi (2016, April 7). “Is The Telltale Games Formula Wearing Thin?” 12 March 2017. http://gamingbolt.com/is-the-telltale-games-formula-wearing-thin
SPIERLING, Ulrike, and Nicolas Szilas (2009). “Authoring Issues beyond Tools.” Interactive Storytelling: Second Joint International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS 2009, Guimarães, Portugal, December 9-11, 2009, Proceedings. Ed. Ido A. Iurgel, Nelson Zagalo, and Paolo Petta. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 5915: 50–61. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-10643-9_9
TELLTALE GAMES. (2012). The Walking Dead [Video game]. San Rafael, CA: Telltale Games.
–––––––––– (2013). The Wolf Amongst us [Video Game]. San Rafael, CA: Telltale Games.
–––––––––– (2015). Minecraft: Story Mode [Video Game]. San Rafael, CA: Telltale Games.
THOMPSON, Kristin, and David Bordwell. (2012). Film Art: An Introduction. New York: McGraw-Hill Education.
VORDERER, Peter, and Leonard Reinecke (2015). “From Mood to Meaning: The Changing Model of the User in Entertainment Research.” Communication Theory, 25.4: 447–453. http://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12082
VORDERER, Peter, Christoph Klimmt, and Ute Ritterfeld (2004). “Enjoyment: At the Heart of Media Entertainment.” Communication Theory, 14.4: 388–408. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2004.tb00321.x
VOSMEER, Mirjam, Christian Roth, and Ben Schouten (2015). “Interaction in Surround Video: The Effect of Auditory Feedback on Enjoyment.” Interactive Storytelling: 8th International Conference on Interactive Digital Storytelling, ICIDS 2015, Copenhagen, Denmark, November 30 - December 4, 2015, Proceedings. Cham: Springer International Publishing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 9445: 202–210. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-27036-4_19
WEIZENBAUM, Joseph (1966). “Eliza — a Computer Program for the Study of Natural Language Communication between Man and Machine.” Communications of the ACM, 9.1:36–45. http://doi.org/10.1145/365153.365168
WREDEN, Davey (2011). The Stanley Parable [Video Game]. Galactic Cafe.
WRIGHT, Peter, and John McCarthy (2008). “Empathy and Experience in HCI.” CHI '08: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM. 637–646. http://doi.org/10.1145/1357054.1357156
WRIGHT, Samantha (2015, November 29). “Why Telltale needs to start reworking its formula for games.” 10 March 2016. http://www.gameskinny.com/tt1x8/why-telltale-needs-to-start-reworking-its-formula-for-games
ZAMMITO, Veronica, Pejman Mirza-Babaei, Ian Livingston, Marina Kobayashi, Lennard E. Nacke (2014). “Player Experience: Mixed Methods and Reporting Results.” CHI '14 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, ACM. 147–150. http://doi.org/10.1145/2559206.2559239
NOTES
[1] In a keynote at ICIDS 2016 conference, Institute for Creative Technologies, November 16, 2016.
© 2018 Hartmut Koenitz, Christian Roth, Teun Dubbelman, Noam Knoller.
Licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 4.0 International (CC BY-NC-ND 4.0).